I really enjoyed reading
“Incognito: The Secret
Lives of the Brain” by neuroscientist
David Eagleman, so much so that
I’ll probably return to it again and again as time goes by.
It is an interesting overview of the current state of our
knowledge about the brain, and Eagleman’s views on the
implications both for society as well as for the individual.
One of his premises is that most of the things that make us who we
are occur below the level of conscious thought. We
already knew that vast swaths of the brain control autonomic behavior,
but Eagleman asserts that more of the things we consider
“us”-including our behavior, beliefs, motivations, and
what we are allowed to think-are learned and burned into the
brain’s circuitry at a level that is simply inaccessible to
conscious inspection, modification, or control.
To paraphrase the popular philosopher
Hamlet,
“There are more things in your speech and behavior,
Horatio,
than are thought up in your consciousness.”
I find this dovetails nicely with the Buddhist
belief that the unexamined life is ruled by
long-established habit patterns from our past, and that most of our
behavior is a straightforward, linear result of the coming together of
conditions: specifically the intersection of those established personality patterns
with the external conditions we find ourselves in.
Amusingly, this echoes something I theorized a good 30 years ago. In a document
I titled “Orny’s Hypotheses”, entry number one reads as follows:
No organized religion can never reflect the true beliefs of its nominal
adherents, for each such individual must learn the tenets of the
religion from an external source and accept them without any possible
reservation. In truth, individuals cannot consciously modify or mold
their beliefs; faith comes from within the individual, and what is in
his heart is his true faith, no matter what his professed faith. This
faith may be discovered through introspection and be consciously
acknowledged or it may remain hidden in the subconscious of the
individual. One cannot decide what one believes, merely discover it,
although this does not prohibit change in beliefs over time.
Getting back to Eagleman, his view of the human mind differs greatly from the popular
conception of a single conscious entity. He regards the brain as what he
terms “a team of rivals”. In his mind, the
brain has different factions, each of which wants to influence the
mind’s single output channel: our behavior. Even the language is
familiar to us: we’re “of two minds” because part of
us wants to eat that bowl of ice cream, but part of us says we
shouldn’t. Rather than a unified single computing machine, the
brain is more like a parliament or a family. But your conscious mind is
only made aware of this when there’s an unresolvable conflict
between factions that requires an arbiter, when a decision needs to be
made.
All this sounds like Eagleman has a dim view of our vaunted
concept of free will. We think we’re in control of our
body and our mind and our personality, but that is largely false.
Freedom-choosing to think and act in ways that are not influenced (if not determined) by our biological, chemical, and material makeup-is an illusion.
Eagleman diverges briefly into a discussion of the
implications this has for criminal justice, based as it is on guilt, blameworthiness, and personal
responsibility. For most people, there is an ethical difference between
a responsible person committing a premeditated crime and someone whose
brain chemistry causes them to perform socially proscribed actions. As
we understand the brain better, our justice system should drop such
outdated concepts as blame, responsibility, and punishment in favor of
altering the criminal’s conditioning and mental habits such that
in the future they will act in accordance with the law.
The thread that most interests me in Eagleman’s book is his
demonstration that who you are and what you think is extremely
closely tied to the chemical and biological state of your
brain. He illustrates how easily the brain can be changed by
various means: narcotics, viruses, genetics, neurotransmitters,
hormones. We tend to think that we all share the same basic brain
function and capacity, but that’s very much not true. We
aren’t even guaranteed that our own brain performs consistently
from day to day. And those changes can have dramatic effects upon our
personality, outlook, opinions, speech, and behavior.
At the same time, Eagleman isn’t a strict material
reductionist. While we are inseparable from our physical componentry, he
views consciousness as a kind of emergent property that might indeed be
something greater than the sum of its parts. But the parts are a whole
lot more important than we’ve been led to believe.
For me, the book prompted a lot of soul-searching (or
mind-searching). It brings up the idea that the ego-the
self-is ultimately nothing more than a very convincing
illusion. In that respect, I must admit that it’s a much
more accessible introduction to that concept than all the esoteric
writings and talks I’ve seen regarding the Buddhist concept of
not-self.
Most people have a visceral reaction against the idea that who we are
is wholly determined by this three-pound bag of neurons. After all, their sense of self is
real and immediate, and giving up that view comes with a very powerful
sense of loss. Perhaps future humans will equate those emotions with what people
felt back in the 17th century when Galileo’s observations
disproved the Ptolomaic view that Earth was the center of the
universe.
Over time, that earlier fall from primacy opened our eyes to the
incomprehensible scale and majesty of the solar system, our galaxy, and
the known universe. If neuroscience winds up evicting our conscious minds from the
central seat of our internal world, it will
simultaneously reveal the brain’s truly incomprehensible
complexity and renew our sense of wonder at the unbelievable
natural achievement that is the human mind.
I’d like to know “what you think”.